ANIMAL PHARMACISTS
https://pnas.org/content/111/49/17339
https://science.sciencemag.org/content/340/6129/150
https://bigthink.com/animals-self-medicate
Research shows sparrows and animals use plants to heal themselves
by Robby Berman / 09 December, 2020
“This week, researchers from China published a study in which an intriguing behavior of russet swallows was described. The birds apparently administer what amounts to preventative medicine to their offspring: wormwood that kills parasites and promotes growth. This suggests two things: that they understand the beneficial properties of the plant, and that they are aware of and invest in their chicks’ future.
Remarkable as this may seem, the sparrows are just the latest addition to an already long list of animals that self-medicate, a capability that raises big questions. Is this behavior learned or is it instinctual? Is the knowledge shared among animal communities and families? Do animals try out different substances until they feel better? Does illness simply induce a physical taste for beneficial plants? Has natural selection favored the survival of animals who just happen to ingest medicinal substances?
The field of study that looks at animals who self-medicate is called zoopharmacognosy. “I believe every species alive today is self-medicating in one way or another,” Michael Huffman of the Primate Research Institute at Kyoto University told the New York Times in 2017. “It’s just a fact of life.” In that New York Times article, Huffman tells the story of a chimp he observed named Chausiku who treated a malaise by chewing the juice from the Vernonia amygdalina plant. According to a local ranger, the plant contains potent medicine but is also deadly at larger doses. Chausiku somehow knew just how much juice to ingest, and she recovered her energy in a few days. She recovered with a powerful appetite, suggesting the resolution of some manner of intestinal distress. Subsequent testing of the plant revealed it has multiple compounds with strong anti-parasitic qualities.
It seems clear that this sort of medicinal savvy is widespread throughout the animal kingdom. A PNAS article was shared by the National Center for Biotechnology Information in 2014. It noted, among other examples:
- Reports of bears, deer, and elk consuming medical plants.
- Elephants in Kenya that induce delivery of their calves by eating certain leaves.
- Lizards that eat a particular anti-venom root when bit by a snake.
- Red and green macaws that ingest clay that calms their digestion (dirt antacids!) and kills bacteria.
- Female wooly spider monkeys in Brazil whose fertility is enhanced by eating certain plants.
It may be primates who are most adept at self-medicating. Chimpanzees, bonobos, and gorillas are often seen swallowing rough leaves that clear their digestive tracts of parasites. Chimps with roundworms will also eat terrible-tasting plants that cure such infestations. Numerous animals—such as the sparrows noted earlier and certain caterpillars—eat plants that kill or repel parasites. Those russet sparrows aren’t the only ones who seem to be planning ahead, either.
There are ants that use antibacterial spruce-tree resin to keep their nests germ free. Finches and sparrow line their nests with cigarette butts that keep mites under control. If science is the practice of making observations, particularly of cause and effect, it may be that these animals are practicing a science of their own. As psychologist Robin Dunbar tell the Times, this method is simply how people and other living beings work out the way things work: “Science is a genuine universal, characteristic of all advanced life-forms.”
An animal’s source of medical knowledge may be as simple as that which comes to an individual with digestive issues who just happens to eat a plant that makes them feel better, a bit of knowledge that will come in handy when it once again gets sick. Perhaps others nearby see what’s happened and learn the trick to recovering from a stomach ache themselves. Perhaps offspring learn the medicine by observing their adults. Emory University’s Jaap de Roode, speaking with NPR, says that “primates are not so different from us. They can learn from each other and they can make associations between … taking medicinal plants and feeling better.”
On the other hand, it could also be natural selection at work. An animal with a natural inclination toward this kind of plant may ingest it when its tummy hurts. It then survives to reproduce while other individuals with tummy aches don’t. The animal uses the plant medicinally without any particular knowledge or understanding. “People used to believe that you had to be very smart to [self-medicate],” says de Roode, but this may not be so. He cites the example of parasite-infected monarch butterflies who will lay their eggs in anti-parasitic milkweed, given the option. “I wouldn’t say it’s a conscious choice, but it’s a choice,” he says, since healthy monarchs don’t exhibit such a preference. However this works, experts say we would be wise to keep an eye on all these non-human practitioners — there may be cures they know about that human physicians haven’t yet caught onto. As de Roode says, animals “have been studying medicine much longer than we have.”
“Dedicated to the memory of Chausiku (ca. 1958 – 1990), my first mentor in chimpanzee self-medication, and a teacher to many of us who have had the privileged of sharing time with Mahale chimpanzees.” (photo by M.A. Huffman, 1987)
ANIMAL SCIENTISTS
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/14506884
https://independent.co.uk/i-know-what-s-good-for-me
https://nytimes.com/the-self-medicating-animal
The Self-Medicating Animal
by Moises Velasquez-Manoff / May 18, 2017
“Chausiku was clearly sick. The chimpanzee was in her 30s, a chimp’s prime. She was usually a gentle, doting mother. But one day she built a nest in a tree, climbed in and lay down, letting her infant, a male named Chopin, roam unsupervised. Another female chimp began looking after Chopin while Chausiku rested. Later that day, after Chausiku descended from the tree, a scientist named Michael Huffman noted that she could barely walk. Huffman wasn’t there to watch Chausiku specifically; he was studying older chimps, researching their social relationships in the troop. But Chausiku did something so out of the ordinary that it captured his attention: She sat down in front of a leafy plant, began pulling off branches, chewing their tips, spitting out the fibrous pulp and swallowing only the juice.
This was in Tanzania, in 1987, when Huffman was just starting his career as a primatologist. In nine months of watching Chausiku’s troop, he had never seen a chimp eat that plant. He asked his friend and research aide, a man named Mohamedi Seifu Kalunde, what it was. Kalunde hailed from the nearby Tongwe people and worked as a ranger at the chimp-research project in Mahale Mountains National Park. The plant Chausiku was chewing, Kalunde told him, was extremely bitter and could be poisonous in large quantities. But it was also “very powerful medicine.” The Tongwe used it to treat stomach ailments and parasite infections. Huffman decided to abandon the other chimps he was trailing and focus on Chausiku. She went to bed early that evening and remained weak the next morning. The other female chimp continued to babysit for Chopin.
But then, after the troop had settled down for its midday rest, Chausiku perked up and hurried away from the group. Huffman and Kalunde struggled to keep up as she moved through the forest, until she reached a spot near the edge of a swamp, where succulents and figs abounded. Chausiku began gorging. “She spends the next few hours stuffing her face as if she were really hungry,” Huffman recalls. When Kalunde told him the plant typically took about 24 hours to work, Huffman felt almost intoxicated by the possibility that he had just seen a chimp use medicine.
That wasn’t the first time scientists had observed what looked like self-medication in the animal kingdom. More than a decade earlier, the primatologist Richard Wrangham and his colleagues noted that chimps often swallowed whole leaves without chewing. The scientists would go on to argue that these chimps were treating parasite infections, and they eventually coined the term zoopharmacognosy — animals that know medicine — to describe the behavior. But that claim met with some skepticism. Whether those leaves actually contained chemical compounds toxic to parasites was unclear. Nor could the researchers prove that these chimps had been sick and became well after self-medicating. Mindful of these shortcomings, Huffman began collecting fecal samples from Chausiku’s troop to get a sense of their parasite loads. Biochemist colleagues analyzed the broad-leafed plant, Vernonia amygdalina, and discovered more than a dozen new compounds with antiparasitic properties.
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=9YuYVYeExdA
After chimps chewed the plant, Huffman would come to learn, parasite loads (as measured by eggs in their stool) decreased by as much as 90 percent in a day. The plant was an effective dewormer. It was also known to kill domestic goats in West Africa, and that toxicity highlighted the apparent skill with which the chimps exploited the plant. Somehow they used enough to kill parasites, but not so much as to kill themselves. They also tended to chew more V. amygdalina during the wet season, when parasites were more abundant, indicating that they sought its medicinal benefits when they needed them most. They seemed to target one parasite in particular, a nasty worm called Oesophagostomum stephanostomum that could cause painful nodules along the intestinal wall and even, in severe infestations, death. Huffman, a professor at Kyoto University in Japan, has since become a leading expert on animal self-medication. In recent decades, primatologists have shown that chimps use tools, know right from wrong and even have the ability, called theory of mind, to imagine what other chimps are experiencing.
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=8p5NyiQgBNY
Huffman’s contribution to this increasingly nuanced understanding of our nearest relative has been to establish that, even though they lack the abilities we consider might be necessary for the development of medical knowledge — namely, humanlike language — chimps practice a form of rudimentary medicine. They know enough about the plants around them to treat illness. Animals of all kinds, from ants and butterflies to sheep and monkeys, use medicine. Certain caterpillars will, when infected by parasitic flies, eat poisonous plants, killing or arresting the growth of the larvae within them. Some ants incorporate resin from spruce trees in their nests to fend off pathogenic microbes, employing the same antibacterial compounds, called terpenes, that we use when we mop the floor with the original Pine-Sol. Parrots and many other animals consume clay to treat an upset stomach; clay binds to toxins, flushing them out of the body. “I believe every species alive today is self-medicating in one way or another,” Huffman told me recently. “It’s just a fact of life.”
Capuchin monkeys use poisonous millipedes and citrus as insect repellent. With howler monkeys, self-medication may veer into social engineering. Ken Glander, an emeritus scientist at Duke, thinks that female howlers sometimes seek out foods that change the acidity of their reproductive organs after mating. By changing the pH balance, he told me, these females may promote the success of male over female sperm, resulting in more male offspring. Should one of those males rise in a troop and sire many children, his mother’s genes are also spread with them. Is what seems to be self-medication simply another instinctual behavior, like the urge to procreate or eat when hungry? Or is it a skill that animals acquire through experience? Most scientists I spoke to pointed out, almost bashfully, that natural selection could produce self-medicating behaviors without the humanlike learning and sharing of expertise that we associate with medical treatment. Animals that happen to eat medicinal plants at the right time might survive more successfully than those that don’t, causing that behavior to spread.
“Mushrooms and lichens begin the decomposition of a fallen tree. Many metabolites are created during this process”
Smaller-brained animals, like caterpillars and ants, are probably self-medicating as a matter of instinct. Even monkeys, with their larger brains, seem to use insect repellents automatically: some drool, writhe and fall into what looks like a trance whenever they encounter a millipede. And yet sheep, which are often considered dimwitted compared to primates, seemingly learn from experience what medicinal plants to draw on and when. There appears to be no hard line in our imagined hierarchy of the animal kingdom, below which self-medication is instinctive and above which medicinal behavior derives from learning. Chimps and other great apes differ, of course, from many other animals. They have culture that we recognize as such — and Huffman considers medical knowledge part of that cultural inheritance. Young chimps closely watch what their mothers eat, and he suspects that this is how they learn what plants to make them better. Chimps in other troops chew different plants than Chausiku did, suggesting that their medicinal knowledge is specific to their environs, not hard-wired. But not everyone thinks the science is settled.
“Red and green macaws have been known to eat kaolin to help with digestion. Clay on riverbeds is said to detoxify the birds by binding to toxic compounds and removing them from their bodies”
Moreover, it’s still unclear how an infant watching its mother learns to associate bitter-tasting plants with physical relief, given that the mother, not the infant, is the one experiencing it and that the effect may not be felt until a day or more after dosing. “That’s the puzzle,” the well-known primatologist and author Frans de Waal told me. And how do they discover medicinal plants to begin with, particularly given their usual bitter taste? “It doesn’t sound logical to me,” he said, “but it must have happened, because we see animals flock to certain resources when they’re sick.” Huffman thinks that people may have acquired medicinal knowledge through the observation of animals. He often tells a story he heard from his friend Kalunde, who died in 2013, to illustrate the point. Kalunde’s grandfather, a healer, once watched a sick porcupine eat the roots of a plant known to be quite poisonous. When the porcupine recovered, Kalunde’s grandfather began experimenting with the root in small doses, first on himself and then on fellow villagers. It turned out to be an effective treatment for dysentery, one the Tongwe still use today.
“Heavily pregnant elephants have been known to eat the plant boraginaceae, which is not part of their regular diet. The same plant is used by women in African regions to induce labour, and it is believed elephants eat the plants to have the same effect”
The ability to observe and imagine the experiences of other animals may have allowed people to tap into ancient, nonhuman medicinal knowledge — “clinical trials that have been ongoing for thousands of years and selected for by natural selection,” Huffman says. “There’s nothing better than that.” The broader implication here is that some animals may be practicing a kind of science, observing cause and effect, experimenting and learning. The University of Oxford evolutionary psychologist Robin Dunbar argues in his book “The Trouble With Science” that not only is science not unique to the West or to the developed world, but that it’s not exclusive to humans. “Science is a genuine universal, characteristic of all advanced life-forms,” he writes. It’s how living things figure out the world around them. To make the case, he cites the behavior of an African bird called the honeyguide, which leads people to beehives with a series of calls, then shares in whatever honeycomb is extracted. Dunbar contends that the honeyguide has developed a theory of human behavior, most likely extrapolated from its relationships with other animals, like the honey badger, which it also leads to beehives.
“Other animals, including birds such as the ruffled goose, apply remedies topically. For example, they grasp ants in their bills and wipe them across their feathers. In particular, ants that spray formic acid are used to kill off feather lice and protect birds from infection.”
Most researchers on animal self-medication demur when asked if the subjects of their study are practicing science, but Juan Villalba, a scientist at Utah State University, thinks that animals practice at least some aspects of what we call the scientific method. He has spent a lot of time watching sheep graze. Sheep know how to choose nutritious food, but like other animals, they regularly eat plants with no obvious nutritional value, perhaps for medicinal reasons. Inspired some years ago by Huffman’s work on chimps, Villalba began looking into how sheep learned which medicinal plants to eat. He started by spiking their regular food with tannins. These organic compounds, responsible for the astringent, mouth-puckering quality of red wine, have evolved in plants to help counter various stresses, including being eaten by animals. They’re meant to taste awful, and sheep usually avoid them. But they also have antiparasitic properties. So Villalba infected lambs with parasite larvae and divided them into two groups; he fed tannin-spiked food to one group and plain feed to the other.
“Pregnant lemurs have been spotted eating tamarind and bark to boost milk production. They also feed the bark to their offspring to help kill parasites”
The parasite loads in animals that ate tannin-rich food dropped relative to those that did not. When Villalba again offered infected animals a choice of tannin-enriched or regular feed, only the lambs that had experienced the bitter forage’s antiparasite effects chose it. Animals that hadn’t cured themselves stuck with regular food and remained infected with parasites. “There’s a learning process that needs to take place in order for them to develop a preference for foods that contain those medicines,” Villalba says. Tellingly, the sheep chose the medicated food only when they required it. When Villalba dewormed the infected animals, even those that had previously medicated themselves successfully avoided the bitter-tasting fare. They sensed that they didn’t need to self-treat. Villalba has also found that lambs and ewes learn about medicinal foods faster when they’re together (rather than alone), as if primed for a transfer of medicinal knowledge. This could explain how, once acquired, medicinal understanding is maintained in groups of animals.
“Goats have been spotted scaling walls in Italy licking stones for their salts and minerals. Known as mineral licks, this behaviour is seen among macaws. Goats use it as a supplement when food supplies are low, or when they are unwell”
The abiding question — the greatest puzzle of all, really — is how animals first learn which plants are medicinal. Villalba has observed that lambs infected with parasites are more likely to try new plants when grazing in an open pasture compared to uninfected lambs. They lose some of what scientists call food “neophobia,” the fear of new flavors, and their greater willingness to explore the surrounding foodscape may increase the odds of a medicinal discovery. Huffman calls these tendencies “pre-adaptations.” They’re hard-wired behaviors that push animals toward the acquisition of medical knowledge — in this case, by impelling them to try the very flavors they normally shun. Arguably, this exploratory behavior exhibits a fundamental insight about the world, which, fully articulated, might go like this: Plants have evolved an exquisite array of poisons and noxious compounds to protect themselves. Many of these are directed at invertebrates and microbes, relatives of what makes an animal sick. So a terrible-tasting plant, one usually avoided, has a better-than-average chance of beating back whatever is making that creature ill.
Monkeys and apes and sheep aren’t performing surgery. They haven’t invented vaccines or figured out how to manufacture insulin. Even older medical traditions, like those in which Mohamedi Kalunde’s grandfather worked — informed by observation and developed through experimentation — are surely far more advanced than what animals manage to do. But they may still provide us with medical insights. Huffman points out, for instance, that while we tend to isolate just one molecule for our drugs, animals use plants that produce multiple antimicrobial and antiparasitic substances at once. By ingesting what’s really a drug cocktail, animals minimize the problem of antibiotic resistance. Villalba’s research may have practical implications. Resistance to antiparasite medication is a growing problem for livestock and people alike.
But if grazing animals are able to medicate themselves, perhaps ranchers should make a wide variety of plants available to them, rather than one or two types of feed. This might allow them to select the medicinal plants they need, improving their health and ultimately saving money spent on medicines. It would also theoretically lessen the risk of antibiotic-resistant superbugs emerging from livestock operations. Anecdotally, Villalba says, ranchers already report that livestock grazing in wilder, more diverse pastures are healthier than those confined to smaller spaces, possibly because they are able to self-medicate. A similar approach might help the honeybees that are so important for crop fertilization and that have been dying at an alarming rate in recent years. Bees collect resins from certain trees to protect themselves against infection, a form of social immunity or communal self-medication.
But these days, with sources of resin scarce in agricultural settings, honeybees may lack those defenses, says Marla Spivak, an entomologist at the University of Minnesota. The unavailability of resins could be one factor making them vulnerable to disease. It’s worth considering the ways that animals, precisely because of their more limited intellects, might be more doggedly scientific than we are. After all, while animals seem to attend closely to cause and effect, learning from experience, people sometimes indulge a penchant for spinning out grand theories from scant (or no) evidence and then acting on them. Bloodletting, for example, persisted for hundreds of years in Europe even though it almost certainly weakened and killed the sick. It was based on the ancient humoral theory of disease: Illness arose when the body’s “humors,” or essential fluids, were out of harmony, an imbalance corrected by draining blood, among other acts. Other ineffectual and even dangerous treatments include smoking to treat asthma and sexual intercourse with virgins as a cure for syphilis.
Animals no doubt blunder in their attempts to self-medicate. But humans seem to be unique in their capacity for clinging to beliefs and theories about the world, even when facing evidence that refutes them. Consider those religious sects that refuse modern medicine altogether, favoring prayer instead, and whose believers sometimes die as a result. Chausiku and her kind would probably never err in this way, simply because the medicine that chimps practice derives from what they’ve learned through trial and error, not from untested explanations for how the world works. Historically, some currents within evidence-based medicine — treatment rigorously based on what has been shown to work — can be regarded as tacit recognition of this human shortcoming. Even modern doctors, with their years of training and conditioning, can find it hard not to venture beyond the evidence or get carried away in extrapolations. In a way, the evidence-based mantra is partly an exhortation to be more animal-like. Don’t rely too heavily on theories, assumptions or grand cosmological narratives. Instead, be empirical and focus on what’s right in front of you.”
ANIMAL DOCTORS
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/21040353
http://cell.com/current-biology/abstract/S0960-9822%2812%2900075-9
https://npr.org/on-call-in-the-wild-animals-play-doctor-too?
On Call In The Wild: Animals Play Doctor, Too
by Audrey Carlsen / April 11, 2013
“A surprising number of wild creatures have figured out ways to use herbs, resins, and even alcohol and nicotine for health’s sake. Scientists review the ranks of animal pharmacists in the latest issue of Science. First on the list: primates, who are particularly good at exploiting the medicinal properties of plants. Chimpanzees, bonobos, and gorillas have all figured out that swallowing rough leaves can purge their intestines of parasites. And chimps plagued by roundworm infections have been known to eat plants with anti-parasitic properties, despite their bitter flavor and lack of nutritional value. These primates seem to know what they’re doing, says Jaap de Roode, an assistant professor of biology at Emory University and lead author of the article. Primates “are not so different from us,” de Roode tells Shots. “They can learn from each other and they can make associations between … taking medicinal plants and feeling better.”
But de Roode points out that not all animals self-medicate consciously. Sometimes, the behavior is innate — the result of natural selection. “People used to believe that you had to be very smart to [self-medicate],” says de Roode. But even insects use substances to keep themselves and their offspring healthy, he says. De Roode’s team studies the behavior of infected monarch butterflies. When given the choice, these infected butterflies will lay their eggs on anti-parasitic milkweed, thus protecting their offspring from infection. Healthy butterflies don’t display this preference. “I wouldn’t say it’s a conscious choice, but it’s a choice,” says de Roode. Plenty of other insects employ strategies to avoid and treat infection. Take the woolly bear caterpillar, for example, which ingests plants that are toxic to parasites. Or the wood ant, which incorporates antimicrobial resin into its nests.
And don’t overlook the crafty fruit fly, which lays its eggs in alcohol from fermented fruit to protect its little ones from parasitic wasps. Birds have a few medicinal tricks up their sleeves too. In cities, house sparrows and finches have been known to add cigarette butts to their nests, which can help to reduce mite infestations. (Nicotine is an effective bug repellent.) According to de Roode, it’s important for scientists to understand how animals defend against parasites: It can affect how we model diseases in wildlife populations. It can also affect human food production. For example, some bees stay healthy by covering their hives in antimicrobial tree resin.
But beekeepers tend to weed out these messier, resin-collecting bees, thus potentially decreasing the overall health of the hive. And, of course, understanding how animals medicate themselves could also lead to insights for human health. “It may actually open our eyes to finding drugs that we could potentially use,” says de Roode. After all, animals “have been studying medicine much longer than we have.”
PREVIOUSLY
DRUGGING the FOOD CHAIN
https://spectrevision.net/2021/07/28/drugging-the-food-chain/
NONHUMAN RIGHTS
https://spectrevision.net/2018/12/19/nonhuman-rights/
SUB-LETHAL METABOLIC EFFECTS
https://spectrevision.net/2018/04/02/sub-lethal-impacts/
https://youtu.be/F_nPsc2EmAE